Bạn đang xem bản rút gọn của tài liệu. Xem và tải ngay bản đầy đủ của tài liệu tại đây (43.93 MB, 1,109 trang )
122
TABLE 3-1
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Neonatal Neurological Examination:
Basic Elements
Level of Alertness
Cranial Nerves
Olfaction (I)
Vision (II)
Optic fundi (II)
Pupils (III)
Extraocular movements (III, IV, VI)
Facial sensation and masticatory power (V)
Facial motility (VII)
Audition (VIII)
Sucking and swallowing (V, VII, IX, X, XII)
Sternocleidomastoid function (XI)
Tongue function (XII)
Taste (VII, IX)
V1
V2
V3
Motor Examination
Tone and posture
Motility and power
Tendon reflexes and plantar response
Primary Neonatal Reflexes
Moro reflex
Palmar grasp
Tonic neck response
Figure 3-1 Dermatomal distribution of the face. Note the delineation
of the three branches of the trigeminal nerve: ophthalmic (V1), maxillary
(V2), and mandibular (V3). (From Enjolras O, Riche MC, Merland JJ:
Facial port-wine stains and Sturge-Weber syndrome, Pediatrics 76:4851, 1985.)
Sensory Examination
Skin. The skin of the head should be examined carefully for the presence of dimples or tracts, subcutaneous masses (e.g., encephalocele, tumor), or cutaneous
lesions, all generally discussed elsewhere in this book.
In this setting, I discuss only the significance of portwine stains, congenital vascular abnormalities that are
present at birth and persist into adulthood. At birth,
these lesions are most often pale pink, macular lesions
that subsequently become dark red to purple and often
nodular.13 Port-wine stains are categorized according
to their dermatomal distribution (Fig. 3-1). Their
importance, apart from the significant cosmetic issue,
relates principally to their association with abnormalities of choroidal vessels in the eye, which may result
TABLE 3-2
in glaucoma, and of meningeal and superficial cerebral
vessels, which may result in cortical lesions with
seizures and other neurological deficits (i.e., SturgeWeber syndrome). The relationships between the
location of the port-wine stain and the incidence
of glaucoma or intracranial vascular lesion are shown
in Table 3-3. In one large series, the intracranial vascular lesion of Sturge-Weber syndrome occurred in
40% to 50% of children with total involvement of
V1.14,15 Notably, with partial involvement of V1, the
risk was markedly lower (1 of 17 children affected),
and none of the 64 children with involvement of V2
or V3, or both, developed either the intracranial
lesion or glaucoma.14,15 The particular prognostic
importance of involvement of V1 was confirmed in
External Characteristics Useful for Estimation of Gestational Age
GESTATIONAL AGE
External
Characteristic
28 Weeks
32 Weeks
36 Weeks
40 Weeks
Ear cartilage
Pinna soft, remains
folded
Pinna harder, springs
back
Pinna firm, stands
erect from head
Breast tissue
External genitalia:
male
None
Testes undescended,
smooth scrotum
1–2-mm nodule
Testes high in scrotum,
more scrotal rugae
External genitalia:
female
Prominent clitoris,
small, widely separated labia
Smooth
Pinna slightly harder
but remains
folded
None
Testes in inguinal
canal, few scrotal
rugae
Prominent clitoris,
larger separated
labia
One or two anterior
creases
6–7-mm nodule
Testes descended,
pendulous scrotum
covered with rugae
Clitoris covered by
labia majora
Plantar surface
Adapted from references 3, 5, and 7 to 10.
Clitoris less prominent,
labia majora cover
labia minora
Two or three anterior
creases
Creases cover sole
Chapter 3
TABLE 3-3
Neurological Examination: Normal and Abnormal Features
123
Relation between Location of Port-Wine Stain and Subsequent Incidence of Glaucoma or SturgeWeber Syndrome
Location of Port-Wine Stain
(Dermatomal Distribution)
Total Number
V1 (total) alone
V1 (total) with other
dermatomes
V1 (partial) with or without
other dermatomes
V2 alone
V3 alone
V2 and V3 (unilateral or
bilateral)
Intracranial Vascular
Lesion with or without
Glaucoma
Glaucoma Alone
Port-Wine Stain Only
4
21
2
9
1
3
1
9
17
1
0
16
29
13
22
0
0
0
0
0
0
29
13
22
Data from Enjolras O, Riche MC, Merland JJ: Facial port-wine stains and Sturge-Weber syndrome, Pediatrics 76:48-51, 1985.
more selected series.16,17 The optimal timing of therapy
has been the subject of debate; a large prospective study
did not report treatment early in infancy and childhood
to be superior to later treatment.18-21
Head Size and Shape. Head circumference is a useful
measure of intracranial volume and therefore also of
volume of brain and cerebrospinal fluid. Less commonly, head circumference is significantly affected
by the size of extracerebral spaces, subdural and subarachnoid, or by the intracranial blood volume. Scalp
edema, subcutaneous infiltration of fluid from
intravenous infusion, and cephalhematoma also have
obvious effects. Nevertheless, measurement of head
circumference remains one of the most readily available
and useful means for evaluating the status of the central
nervous system in the newborn period. Longitudinal measurements in particular provide valuable
information.
Head circumference is influenced by head shape: the
more circular the head shape, the smaller the circumference needs to be to contain the same area and the
same intracranial volume. Infants with relatively large
occipital-frontal diameters have larger measured head
circumferences than infants with relatively large biparietal diameters. This finding has important implications for evaluating the head circumference of an
infant with a skull deformity such as craniosynostosis
(see next paragraph). In premature infants, over the first
2 to 3 months of life, an impressive change in head
shape is characterized by an increase in occipital-frontal diameter relative to biparietal diameter (Fig. 3-2).
Because this alteration occurs over a matter of weeks,
it usually does not cause major difficulties in the interpretation of head circumference, but it does need to be
considered, especially in infants with unusually marked
dolichocephalic change.
Craniosynostosis, defined as premature cranial suture
closure, may affect one or more cranial sutures (Table
3-4).22 Simple sagittal synostosis is most common.22,23
The diagnosis can be suspected by the shape of the
head; with synostosis of a suture, growth of the skull
can occur parallel to the affected suture but not at right
angles (Fig. 3-3). The ‘‘keel-shaped’’ head of sagittal
synostosis is termed dolichocephaly or scaphocephaly, the
wide head of coronal synostosis is brachycephaly, and the
tower-shaped head of combined coronal, sagittal, and
lambdoid synostosis is acrocephaly. A few cases of
AP/BP ratio
1.3
AP/BP ratio
1.48
AP/BP ratio
1.54
Figure 3-2 Change in head shape in premature infants. Measurements of AP/BP
ratio (anterior-posterior [AP] and biparietal
[BP] diameters) and drawings of head
shape (vertex view) of an infant, born at
28 weeks of gestation, made at, A, 1
week, B, 5 weeks, and, C, 111=2 weeks.
(From Baum JD, Searls D: Head shape
and size of pre-term low-birthweight infants,
Med Child Neurol 13:576-581, 1971.)
A
B
C
124
TABLE 3-4
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Coronal
Distribution of Suture Involvement in
Craniosynostosis
Sutures
Sagittal only
Coronal only
One
Both
Metopic only
Lambdoid only (one or both)
Various combinations
Percentage of
Cases*
Lambdoid
56
25
13
12
4
2
13
*Total of 519 patients.
Data from Matson D: Neurosurgery of Infancy and Childhood,
Springfield, IL: 1969, Charles C Thomas.
cranial synostosis represent complex syndromes,
the major features, genetics, and neurological outcome
of which are summarized in Table 3-5.24-30 The importance of early correction of synostosis for optimal
cosmetic appearance and the other aspects of management are discussed in standard textbooks of
neurosurgery.
Positional or deformational plagiocephaly has become a
frequent clinical issue. The term plagiocephaly (‘‘oblique
head,’’ from the Greek) refers to a head appearance in
which the occipital region is flattened and the ipsilateral frontal area is prominent (i.e., anteriorly displaced)
(Fig. 3-4). In positional or deformational plagiocephaly,
caused by external molding forces, the ipsilateral ear is
also displaced anteriorly, and the contralateral side of
the face may appear flattened.31,32 Torticollis may be
associated and may cause a head tilt. Deformation plagiocephaly may be present at birth, secondary to intrauterine restriction to head movement as occurs with
multiple gestation, abnormal uterine lie, or neck
abnormality (e.g., torticollis), or it may evolve over the
first weeks to months of life, usually secondary to supine
sleeping position as part of the ‘‘Back to Sleep’’
program.31,33 Differentiation of deformational plagiocephaly from the rare unilateral lambdoid synostosis,
which can also cause occipital flattening, is usually
readily made clinically. In unilateral lambdoid
synostosis, the anterior displacement of the frontal area
is usually less pronounced, the ear is posterior, not
anterior, and is displaced inferiorly, and facial
deformity is rare. Management of deformational
plagiocephaly consists of parental counseling regarding head positioning with the infant supine,
supervised time in the prone position, various
exercises, and skull-molding helmets if necessary
(see Fig. 3-4).31,34-36
Rate of Head Growth
Interpretation of the rate of head growth in premature
infants is often difficult, in part because ‘‘normal’’ postnatal rates have been difficult to define conclusively (in
contrast to normal rates of intrauterine growth, as
plotted on most standard charts) and in part because
commonly occurring systemic diseases and caloric
deprivation in the neonatal period may interfere with
brain and head growth.
Metopic
Coronal
Sagittal
Lambdoid
A
Metopic
B
C
Figure 3-3 Changes related to premature closure of cranial sutures.
Schematic diagram of, A, cranial sutures and changes in cranial shape
with premature closure of, B, sagittal or, C, coronal sutures.
The rate of head growth in premature infants has
been the subject of numerous reports.37-50 In the
healthy premature infant, change in the head circumference in the first days of life is minimal; indeed, a small
amount of head shrinkage with suture overriding has
been documented.39,51 Head shrinkage reaches a peak
at approximately 3 days of life, usually averages 2% to
3% of the head circumference at birth, and correlates
closely with postnatal weight and urinary sodium
losses. In view of these findings and the overriding of
Chapter 3
TABLE 3-5
125
Craniosynostosis Syndromes
Syndrome
Name
Crouzon
Carpenter
Apert
SaethreChotzen
Pfeiffer
Antley-Bixler
Greig
Baller-Gerold
Opitz
Neurological Examination: Normal and Abnormal Features
Cranium
Other Major Features
Acrocephaly (tower-shaped) Ocular proptosis (shallow
orbits) and maxillary
with synostosis of
hypoplasia
coronal, sagittal, and
lambdoid sutures
Acrobrachycephaly with
Lateral displacement of
synostosis of coronal,
inner canthi, polydactyly
sagittal, and lambdoid
and syndactyly of feet
sutures
Brachycephaly with irregu- Midfacial hypoplasia,
syndactyly of fingers and
lar synostosis, espetoes, broad distal
cially of coronal suture
phalanx of thumb
and big toe
Brachycephaly with synos- Prominent ear crus, maxiltosis of coronal suture
lary hypoplasia, partial
syndactyly of fingers and
toes
Brachycephaly with synos- Hypertelorism, broad
tosis of coronal and/or
thumbs and toes, partial
sagittal sutures
syndactyly of fingers and
toes
Brachycephaly with multiple Maxillary hypoplasia, radiosynostosis, especially of
humeral synostosis,
coronal suture
choanal atresia,
arthrogryposis
High forehead with variable Hypertelorism, polydactyly
synostosis
and syndactyly of fingers
and toes
Radial dysplasia with
Synostosis of variable
absent thumbs
sutures, including
metopic with
trigonocephaly
Trigonocephaly with
Upward slant of palpebral
synostosis of metopic
fissures, epicanthal
suture
folds, narrow palate,
anomalies of external
ear, loose skin, variable
polydactyly or syndactyly
of fingers
sutures, investigators have suggested that the head
shrinkage relates to water loss from the intracranial
compartment.39
A longitudinal study of 41 premature infants
(<1500 g birth weight) with favorable neurological outcome at age 2 years, as assessed by neurological examination and Bayley Mental Developmental Scale,
defined the rates of head growth shown in Table 3-6.
Thus, after a period of decreasing head circumference
in the first week, head growth increased by a mean of
approximately 0.50 cm in the second week, 0.75 cm in
the third week, and 1.0 cm per week thereafter in the
neonatal period. Slower rates of head growth were
observed in infants with serious systemic disorders
and subsequent neurological impairment.41 Faster
rates of head growth in the first 6 weeks suggest hydrocephalus (e.g., after intraventricular hemorrhage), as
detailed in Chapter 11.38,41 ‘‘Sick’’ preterm infants
with systemic disease often exhibit a ‘‘normal’’ acceleration of head growth (i.e., ‘‘catch-up’’ head growth)
after recovery from their illness.52 However, the
Genetics
Neurological Outcome
Autosomal dominant
(variable
expression)
Mental retardation
occasional
Autosomal recessive
Mental retardation
common
Autosomal dominant
(usually new
mutation)
Mental retardation or
borderline intelligence
common
Autosomal dominant
(variable
expression)
Mental retardation
uncommon
Autosomal dominant
Normal intelligence
usual
Autosomal recessive
Intelligence probably
normal
Autosomal dominant
Mild mental retardation
occasional
Autosomal recessive
Mental retardation
common
Autosomal recessive
Mental retardation
common
smallest infants, those who weigh less than 1000 g at
birth, generally do not exhibit as rapid growth as premature infants whose birth weight is greater than
2000 g and do not catch up even by 2 years of age.45
Additionally, preterm infants born small for their gestational age often do not exhibit as rapid head growth
or as effective catch-up growth as infants born average
for their gestational age.48
The importance of duration of neonatal caloric deprivation (<85 kcal/kg/day) on head growth in the neonatal period was shown in a study of 73 preterm infants
(mean gestational age, 30 ± 2 weeks) (Fig. 3-5).53 Three
phases of head growth were defined: an initial period of
growth arrest or suboptimal head growth, followed by a
period of catch-up growth, and terminated by a period
of growth along standard curves. The duration of the
period of growth arrest or suboptimal growth was
directly related to the initial period of caloric deprivation and to the duration of mechanical ventilation, and
the period of catch-up growth was directly related to
the duration of the preceding caloric deprivation only.
126
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Frontal prominence
+2 S
Head circumference (cm)
46
D
42
D
–2 S
38
3
34
30
2
26
1
22
30
Flattening
Ipsilateral ear
displaced
anteriorly
Contralateral
occipital
prominence
Figure 3-4 Positional or deformational plagiocephaly. Note the flattening of the right occiput, because the infant is placed primarily in a
supine position and the infant’s preferred head position is to the right.
The other changes are described in the text.
The rate of head growth along standard curves was
between the mean and 1 standard deviation (SD)
below the mean for all infants except those calorically
deprived the longest (4 to 6 weeks), in whom values
were more than 1 SD below the mean. Indeed, such
infants calorically deprived for more than 4 weeks had
developmental scores lower than normal ranges at
1 year of corrected age. The deleterious effect of postnatal caloric deprivation is worse for preterm infants
born small for their gestational age.48
Level of Alertness
The formal neonatal neurological examination should
begin with assessment of the level of alertness. The level
of alertness is perhaps the most sensitive of all neurological functions because it depends on the integrity of
several levels of the central nervous system (see later).
Terms used to describe this aspect of neurological
function include state54,55 and vigilance56 (Table 3-7).
TABLE 3-6
Rates of Head Growth in Premature Infants
with Favorable Neurological Outcome
Postnatal Week
First
Second
Third
After third
Rate of Head Growth
(cm/week)
–0.60
0.50
0.75
1.0
Data from a study of 41 premature infants (<1500 g birth weight) in
Gross SJ, Oehler JM, Eckerman CO: Head growth and developmental outcome in very low-birth-weight infants, Pediatrics 71:70-75,
1983.
34
Gestational
age (wk)
40
3
6
9
Postterm
age (mo)
Figure 3-5 Phases of head growth. Phases of head growth derived
from data of Georgieff and co-workers and based on study of 73
premature infants of 30±2 weeks of gestation (mean ± 2 SDs). The
three phases shown are discussed in the text. (From Georgieff MD,
Hoffman JS, Pereira GR, Bernbaum J, et al: Effect of neonatal caloric
deprivation on head growth and 1-year development status in preterm
infants, J Pediatr 107:581-587, 1985.)
The first two states correspond to quiet and active
sleep, respectively (see Chapter 4), and the next three
states describe different levels of wakefulness. The level
of alertness in the normal infant varies, depending particularly on time of last feeding, environmental stimuli,
recent experiences (e.g., painful venipuncture), and
gestational age.57-61 Before 28 weeks of gestation, it is
difficult to identify periods of wakefulness. Persistent
stimulation leads to eye opening and apparent alerting
for time periods measured principally in seconds. At
approximately 28 weeks, however, a distinct change
occurs in the level of alertness.56 At that time,
a gentle shake rouses the infant from apparent
sleep and results in alerting for several minutes.
Spontaneous alerting also occasionally occurs at this
age. Sleep-waking cycles are difficult to observe clinically but can be shown electrophysiologically.62 By
32 weeks, stimulation is no longer necessary; frequently, the eyes remain open, and spontaneous
roving eye movements appear. Sleep-waking alternation, as defined by clinical observation, is apparent.56
By 36 weeks, increased alertness can be observed readily, and vigorous crying appears during wakefulness. By
term, the infant exhibits distinct periods of attention to
visual and auditory stimuli, and it is possible to study
sleep-waking patterns in detail.58,61,63-67
Cranial Nerves
Olfaction (I)
Olfaction, a function subserved by the first cranial nerve,
is evaluated only rarely in the newborn period. In a
study of 100 term and preterm infants, Sarnat observed
that all normal infants of more than 32 weeks of gestation responded with sucking, arousal-withdrawal, or
both, to a cotton pledget soaked with peppermint
Chapter 3
TABLE 3-7
State
1
2
3
4
5
Neurological Examination: Normal and Abnormal Features
127
Major Features of Neonatal Behavioral States in Term Infants
Eyes Open
Respiration Regular
Gross Movements
Vocalization
–
–
+
+
±
+
–
+
–
–
–
±
–
+
+
–
–
–
–
+
–, absent; +, present; ±, present or absent.
Data from Prechtl HFR, O’Brien, MJ: Behavioural states of the full-term newborn: The emergence of a concept. In Stratton P, editor: Psychobiology of
the Human Newborn, New York: 1982, John Wiley & Sons.
extract.68 Eight of 11 infants of 29 to 32 weeks of gestation, but only one of six infants of 26 to 28 weeks of
gestation also responded. Activation of orbitofrontal,
olfactory cortex was detected by near-infrared spectroscopy in full-term newborns exposed to vanilla or
maternal colostrum in the first weeks of life.69
Olfactory Discriminations. More sophisticated techniques have demonstrated olfactory discriminations in
newborns.70,71 Using habituation-dishabituation techniques and recordings of respiration, heart rate, and
motor activity, Lipsitt and colleagues demonstrated
detection and discrimination among a variety of odorants.72-74 Mediation of discriminations at a higher level
than the periphery was shown by the observation that
infants, initially habituated to mixtures of odorants,
exhibited dishabituation when presented with the
pure components of the mixtures. A particularly interesting demonstration of olfactory discrimination in the
infant involved discrimination of the odor of breast
pads of the infant’s mother from unused pads or
those of other nursing mothers.70,75 Infants consistently adjusted their faces and gazes toward the
pads of their own mothers. Later work involving
coupling of stroking with different odorants demonstrated complex associative olfactory learning in the
first 48 hours of life.70,76 That olfactory discrimination
develops in utero is suggested by the demonstration
of neonatal preference for the odors of amniotic
fluid.77 Finally, nutrient (breast milk or formula) odor
exposure through a pacifier was shown to stimulate
nonnutritive sucking during gavage feeding of premature newborns.78
Vision (II)
Visual responses, the afferent segment of which is subserved by the second cranial nerve, exhibit distinct
changes with maturation in the neonatal period. By
26 weeks, the infant consistently blinks to light.56,79
By 32 weeks, light provokes eye closure that persists
for as long as the light is present (dazzle reflex of
Peiper).80 More sophisticated studies indicate that a
series of behaviors associated with visual fixation can
be identified by 32 weeks of gestation and can be
shown to increase considerably over the next
4 weeks.81 By 34 weeks, more than 90% of infants
will track a fluffy ball of red wool.82 At 37 weeks, the
infant will turn the eyes toward a soft light.56 By term,
visual fixation and following are well developed.83-85
For testing of visual fixation and following, I have
found a most useful target to be a fluffy ball of red
yarn. Opticokinetic nystagmus, elicited by a rotating
drum, is present in the majority of infants at 36
weeks and is present consistently at term.80,84,86,87
The anatomical substrate for visual fixation and for
following a moving object in the newborn may not be
primarily the occipital cortex, as usually thought. Thus,
two studies of newborn infants with apparent absence
of occipital cortex secondary to maldevelopment (holoprosencephaly) or destructive lesion (congenital hydrocephalus, ischemic injury) suggested that these abilities
are mediated at subcortical sites.88-90 Experimental studies in subhuman primates defined such a subcortical
system involving retina, optic nerves and tract, pulvinar, and superior colliculus—the so-called collicular
visual system.91,92 Visual abilities beyond the ability to
track a moving object (i.e., visual discriminatory skills;
see the following paragraphs), however, do require the
geniculocalcarine cortical system.
Visual Acuity, Color, and Other Discriminations.
Elegant studies provided important information about
neonatal visual acuity, color perception, contrast sensitivity,
and visual discrimination.85,93-102 Through use of the opticokinetic nystagmus response to striped patterns of
varying width, investigators demonstrated that the
newborn exhibits at least 20/150 vision.103 Using a
visual fixation technique, Fantz showed that the newborn attended to stripes of 1/8-inch width.104 Visual
acuity in premature infants with birth weights of 1500
to 2500 g who were studied at approximately 38 weeks
of gestational age was similar to that of term infants.99
Although studies of color perception in the newborn
period often have not rigorously distinguished brightness and color, newborn infants clearly follow a colored
object.105 Color vision is demonstrable by at least as
early as 2 months of age.106,107 Contrast sensitivity
increases dramatically between 4 and 9 postnatal
weeks.96
Discrimination of a rather complex degree has been
demonstrated for newborn infants.85,94,97,98,108-114
Infants as young as 35 weeks of gestation exhibit a distinct visual preference for patterns, particularly those
with a greater number of and larger details. Curved
contours are favored over straight lines. Preference for
novel patterns becomes apparent at 3 to 5 months.98
Preference for patterns with facial resemblance
develops between approximately 10 and 15 weeks of
age,115 and promptly thereafter discrimination occurs
according to facial features.116 The degree of contrast
128
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
has a direct effect on preferences.117 Binocular vision
and appreciation of depth also appear by approximately
3 to 4 postnatal months.94 Binocular visual acuity
increases most rapidly during the same interval.95
These higher-level visual abilities may reflect a
change in the major anatomical substrate from subcortical to cortical structures.85,92,118 Nevertheless, two
studies of infants from the first days of life by functional
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) did show some
evidence for activation of the visual cortex with visual
stimulation; subcortical structures could not be
addressed because of small anatomical size.119,120
Infants in the first days of life also have been shown
to imitate facial gestures (Fig. 3-6).121,122 Additionally,
imitation of finger movements, especially involving
the left hand, has been demonstrated in healthy
term infants.123 Thus, striking changes in cortically
mediated visual function occur in the first weeks
and months of postnatal life. This is a period for
rapid dendritic growth and synaptogenesis in visual
cortex and myelination of the optic radiation (see
Chapter 2).
Optic Fundi (II)
The funduscopic examination in the newborn period is
facilitated considerably by the aid of a nurse and
patience on the part of the examiner. The optic disc of
the newborn lacks much of the pinkish color observed
in the older infant and has a paler, gray-white appearance. This color and the less prominent vascularity of
the neonatal optic disc may make distinction from optic
atrophy difficult. Retinal hemorrhages have been observed
in 20% to 40% of all newborn infants, with no association with obvious perinatal difficulties, concomitant
central nervous system injury, or neurological sequelae.80,124-126 A relationship with vaginal delivery is
apparent; in one study, 38% of infants delivered vaginally exhibited retinal hemorrhages, in contrast to 3% of
infants delivered by cesarean section (Table 3-8).125
The hemorrhages generally resolve completely within
7 to 14 days. Consistent with these findings, an evaluation of eight consecutive newborns with retinal
hemorrhages by MRI scan revealed no intracranial
abnormalities.127
Pupils (III)
The pupils are sometimes difficult to evaluate in the
newborn, especially the premature baby, because the
eyes are often closed and resist forced opening, and
the poorly pigmented iris provides poor contrast for
visualizing the pupil. The size of the pupils in the premature infant is approximately 3 to 4 mm and is slightly
greater in the full-term infant. Reaction to light begins
to appear at approximately 30 weeks of gestation, but it
is not present consistently until approximately 32 to
35 weeks.79,128 The amplitude of the pupillary response
increases markedly between 30 weeks and term
(Fig. 3-7).129 The afferent arc of this reflex leaves the
optic tract before the lateral geniculate nucleus and
synapses in the pretectal region of midbrain before
innervating the Edinger-Westphal nucleus of the oculomotor nerve, the efferent arc of the reflex.
Figure 3-6 Imitation of facial gestures. Sample photographs from videotape recordings of 2- to 3-week-old infants imitating tongue protrusion,
mouth opening, and lip protrusion demonstrated by an adult experimenter. (From Meltzoff AN, Moore MK: Imitation of facial and manual gestures by
human neonates, Science 198:74-78, 1977.)
Chapter 3
TABLE 3-8
Neonatal Retinal Hemorrhage:
Influence of Perinatal Factors
RETINAL HEMORRHAGES
No. Affected/
Total No.
Perinatal Factor
Normal vaginal delivery
Abnormal vaginal delivery
Vaginal delivery
Spontaneous
Forceps
Cesarean section
Affected (%)
48/127
22/69
38
32
61/160
9/36
1/38
38
25
3
Data from Besio R, Caballero C, Meerhoff E, Schwarcz R: Neonatal
retinal hemorrhages and influence of perinatal factors, Am J
Ophthalmol 87:74-76, 1979.
Extraocular Movements (III, IV, VI)
Particular attention should be paid to eye position,
spontaneous eye movements, and movements elicited
by the doll’s eyes maneuver, vertical spin, or caloric
stimulation, as well as to a variety of abnormal eye
movements (see later). These oculomotor functions
are subserved by cranial nerves III, IV, and VI and
their interconnections within the brain stem. In most
premature and some full-term infants, the eyes are
slightly disconjugate at rest, with one or the other 1
to 2 mm out. (This feature is demonstrated readily by
observing the light reflected off each pupil with the
4.5
Pupil diameter
in relative darkness
3.5
3.0
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
Net pupil
response to light
0.5
0
26
28
30
32
34
36
38
Postconceptional age (wk)
129
light source in the midline at approximately 2 feet
from the face.)
As early as 25 weeks of gestation, full ocular movement with the doll’s eyes maneuver can be elicited.
Because interfering ocular fixation is not well developed at this stage, elicitation of lateral eye movements
with the doll’s eyes maneuver is much easier in the
small premature infant than in the full-term infant.
Another convenient means of eliciting oculovestibular
responses is to spin the baby held upright; the eyes
deviate in a direction opposite to the spin. Rapid maturation of this response with development of nystagmus as well as eye deviation occurs in the first
2 postnatal months.130 Additionally, at 30 weeks of
gestation, caloric stimulation with cold water leads to
deviation of the eyes toward the side of the stimulated
ear.131 Spontaneous roving eye movements are common
at approximately 32 weeks.132 The tracking movements
of the full-term and older infants at first are rather jerky
and do not become smooth and gliding until approximately the third month of life.80
Facial Sensation and Masticatory Power (V)
Subserved by cranial nerve V (i.e., the trigeminal nerve),
facial sensation is examined best with pinprick. The
resulting facial grimace begins on the stimulated side
of the face. If the infant has facial palsy, this response
will be impaired and may be attributed mistakenly to
involvement of the trigeminal nerve or nucleus. The
strength of masseters and pterygoids also depends on
the function of the trigeminal nerve. This strength is
assessed by evaluation of sucking and by allowing the
infant to bite down on the examiner’s finger.
Facial Motility (VII)
The parameters of interest are the position of the face at
rest, the onset of movement, and the amplitude and
symmetry of spontaneous and elicited movement.
Facial motility is subserved by cranial nerve VII. While
the infant’s face is at rest, attention should be paid to the
vertical width of the palpebral fissure, the nasolabial
fold, and the position of the corner of the mouth.
Examination of the face should not be restricted to
observation of elicited movements (e.g., crying) because
the quality of spontaneous facial movement is of greatest
importance in the assessment of cerebral lesions. Subtle
lesions at all central levels are best detected by close
observation of the onset of movement.
5.0
4.0
Neurological Examination: Normal and Abnormal Features
40
42
Figure 3-7 Pupil diameter in relation to light. Diameter of pupil in
millimeters (mean ± SD) in term and preterm neonates in relative
darkness (<10 foot-candles) and after light stimulation (600 foot-candles). (From Isenberg SJ: Clinical application of the pupil examination in
neonates, J Pediatr 118:650-652, 1991.)
Audition (VIII)
The eighth cranial nerve, through its connections in
the brain stem and cerebral cortex, subserves auditory
function. By 28 weeks, the infant startles or blinks to a
sudden, loud noise.56 As the infant matures, more
subtle responses become evident, such as cessation of
motor activity, change in respiratory pattern, opening
of mouth, and wide opening of eyes.80 The relation of
such responses to the development of hearing has been
the subject of considerable study and controversy, but
these responses likely represent the presence of at least
some auditory function. Inability to elicit these
responses is related usually to the failure to test in a
130
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
quiet surrounding, while the baby is alert and not agitated or very hungry, and to ensure that the ear canals
are free of the often copious vernix. In most cases, an
infant who does not respond on the initial examination
will respond when retested under more favorable conditions. More detailed evaluation of auditory function,
including electrophysiological measurements (e.g.,
brain stem auditory evoked responses; see Chapter 4),
certainly is indicated if behavioral responses are consistently absent.
respond to normal speech with activation of the
temporal regions preferentially in the left hemisphere.151,152 These interesting observations demonstrate that the newborn brain exhibits the cortical
organization to process speech and the regional
specification for the left hemisphere for language.
Similarly, a magnetoencephalographic study using a
paradigm based on sound discrimination and important in auditory cognitive function demonstrated positive responses in newborns shortly after birth.153
Auditory Acuity, Localization, and Discriminations. More sophisticated studies have provided insight
into neonatal auditory acuity, localization, and discriminations. Using the occurrence in the newborn of cardiac acceleration in relation to sound intensity,
Steinschneider demonstrated a threshold for cardiac
acceleration of approximately 40 decibels.133 Auditory
localization has been shown by demonstrating loss and
recovery of habituation to an auditory stimulus
by changing the locus of the stimulus.134,135 Auditoryvisual coordination in localization was shown by exposing
an infant to the mother speaking before the infant
through a soundproof glass screen, with her voice transmitted by a stereo system.136,137 When the stereo system
was in balance (i.e., the voice came from straight ahead),
the infant was content, but when the voice appeared to
come from a location different from that of the face, the
infant became very upset. Maturation of connections
between brain stem auditory nuclei (superior olivary
nucleus, nucleus of lateral lemniscus, inferior colliculus), sensory nuclei, and facial nerve nucleus has been
studied by measurement of the amplitude of the blink
response to glabellar tap when the tap is preceded by an
auditory tone.138,139
Through the use of heart rate patterns and a
habituation-dishabituation model, it has been possible
to demonstrate auditory discriminations in 3- to 5-day-old
newborn infants on the basis of intensity, pitch, and
rhythm. These findings are of particular interest in
view of information suggesting that intensity and pitch
discriminations may be mediated at subcortical levels,
whereas cortical levels are required for discrimination of
temporal patterns.140 Discrimination of synthetic
speech sounds according to phonemic category and
of tonal sounds of different frequencies was demonstrated in newborns in the first days of life.141-144
Discrimination of real and computer-simulated cries
by newborn infants was shown by observing much restlessness and crying in infants stimulated by the real cry
and considerably less such behavior in those stimulated
by the computer-simulated cry.145,146 Moreover, results
of other studies indicate a preference of the newborn for
human voice rather than nonhuman sounds147 and particular preference for the mother’s voice rather than
another human voice.148,149 Finally, 2- to 4-week-old
infants can learn to recognize a word that their mothers
repeat to them over a period of time (2 weeks) and
‘‘remember’’ the word for up to 2 days without
intervening presentations.150
Studies based on optical topography or functional
MRI show that newborns in the first days of life
Sucking and Swallowing (V, VII, IX, X, XII)
Sucking requires the function of cranial nerves V, VII,
and XII,80,154 swallowing requires cranial nerves IX and
X, and tongue function uses cranial nerve XII. The
importance of tongue function, particularly the
‘‘stripping’’ action of the medial tongue, was demonstrated in ultrasonographic and fiberoptic studies of neonatal feeding.155-158 The act of feeding requires the
concerted action of breathing, sucking, and swallowing.157,159-163 Not surprisingly, the brain stem control
centers for these actions, termed pattern generators, are
closely situated.163,164 Sucking and swallowing are coordinated sufficiently for oral feeding as early as
28 weeks.132 This finding perhaps is not surprising
because swallowing is observed in utero as early as
11 weeks of gestation.165 The development of rooting
at approximately 28 weeks is a relevant complementing
feature. At this early age, however, the synchrony of
breathing with sucking and swallowing is not well developed,157 and thus oral feeding is difficult and, in fact,
dangerous. By 34 weeks of gestation, however, the
normal infant is able to maintain a concerted synchronous action for productive oral feeding.161,163,166
However, maturation continues rapidly, and linkage of
breathing, sucking, and swallowing is not achieved fully
until 37 weeks of gestation or more.157,163 Moreover,
even in the healthy term infant, coordination of swallowing and breathing rhythms is not optimal in the first
48 hours of life.160
The gag reflex, subserved by cranial nerves IX and X,
is an important part of the neurological evaluation in this
context. A small tongue blade or a cotton-tipped swab
can be used to elicit the reflex. Active contraction of the
soft palate, with upward movement of the uvula and of
the posterior pharyngeal muscles, should be observed.
Sternocleidomastoid Function (XI)
Function of the sternocleidomastoid muscle is mediated
by cranial nerve XI. Because the function of the muscle
is to flex and rotate the head to the opposite side, it is
difficult to test in the newborn, especially in the premature infant. One useful maneuver with the full-term
infant is to extend the head gently over the side of the
bed with the child in the supine position. Passive
rotation of the head reveals the configuration and bulk
of the muscle, and function sometimes can be estimated
if the infant attempts to flex the head.
Tongue Function (XII)
Function of tongue is mediated by cranial nerve XII.
The parameters of interest are the size and symmetry of
Chapter 3
the muscle, the activity at rest, and the movement.
Tongue movement is assessed best during the infant’s
sucking on the examiner’s fingertip. The important
role of the tongue in oral feeding was discussed in
relation to sucking and swallowing.
Taste (VII, IX)
Taste is evaluated only rarely in the neonatal neurological examination. This function is subserved by cranial
nerves VII (anterior two thirds of tongue) and IX (posterior one third of tongue). The newborn infant is very
responsive to variations in taste and is capable of sharp
discriminations. Lipsitt and co-workers used various
parameters of sucking behavior, not only to define gustatory discriminations but also to study learning processes in the newborn.167,168 An apparatus that allows
control of the fluid to be obtained by sucking, as well as
measurement of duration and frequency of sucking,
has been used to demonstrate that, when presented
with a sweet fluid (e.g., 15% sucrose), the infant
sucks in longer bursts and with fewer rest periods
than when presented with water or a salty fluid.167-169
When sucking the sweet fluid, the heart rate increased.
It was presumed from these data that the newborn
infant ‘‘hedonically monitors oral stimuli and signals
the pleasantness of such stimuli with the heart rate as
an indicator response.’’74
Motor Examination
The major features of the motor examination to be
evaluated in the neonatal period are muscle tone and
the posture of limbs, motility and muscle power, and
the tendon reflexes and plantar response. The postnatal
age and level of alertness of the infant have an important bearing on essentially all these features. Most of
the observations described next are applicable to an
infant of more than 24 hours of age and in an optimal
level of alertness, unless otherwise indicated.
Tone and Posture
Muscle tone is assessed best by passive manipulation of
limbs with the head placed in the midline. Moreover,
because tone of various muscles in part determines the
posture of the limbs at rest, careful observation of posture is valuable for the proper evaluation of tone. Some
investigators have devised various maneuvers of passive
manipulation of limbs (e.g., approximation of heel to
ear, hand to opposite ear [scarf sign], or measurement
of angles of certain joints, such as the popliteal angle)
to attempt to quantitate tone.4,11,12,170 These maneuvers have not been particularly useful for me and are
not discussed in detail.
Developmental Aspects. Saint-Anne Dargassies and
co-workers described an approximate caudal-rostral
progression in the development of tone, particularly
flexor tone, with maturation.56 At 28 weeks, resistance
to passive manipulation is minimal in all limbs, but by
32 weeks, distinct flexor tone becomes apparent in
the lower extremities. By 36 weeks, flexor tone is prominent in the lower extremities and is palpable in the
Neurological Examination: Normal and Abnormal Features
131
upper extremities. By term, passive manipulation affords
appreciation of strong flexor tone in all extremities.
The posture of the infant in repose reflects these
changes in tone to some extent. In my experience,
these postures are apparent principally when the
infant is in a slightly drowsy state. The alert infant at
these various gestational ages is more active and motile,
and fixed postures or so-called preference postures are
difficult to define. This finding was well documented
by Prechtl and co-workers and by others.171-175
Nevertheless, the very quiet infant at 28 weeks often
lies with minimally flexed limbs, whereas by 32 weeks
one notes distinct flexion of the lower extremities at the
knees and hips. By 36 weeks, flexor tone in the lower
extremities results in a popliteal angle of 90%, and consistent and frequent flexion occurs at the elbows. By
term, the infant assumes a flexed posture of all
limbs.56 Fisting, usually bilateral, is the predominant
hand posture.56,176 The evolution of hip (and knee)
flexor tone with maturation is reflected in the developmental increase in pelvic elevation when the infant is in
the prone position.177
Preference of Head Position. A consistent and interesting aspect of posture in newborn infants is a preference for position of the head toward the right
side.171,178-180 Prechtl and co-workers demonstrated
head position toward the right side 79% of the time
versus 19% toward the left and 2% toward the midline
(Fig. 3-8).171 In one study, this preference increased
with gestational age,179 whereas in another it decreased.180 The head orientation preference may be
less prominent in the first 24 hours of life.181 This preference has not been attributable to differences in lighting, nursing practices, or other factors, but it appears to
reflect a normal asymmetry of cerebral function at this
age. Notably the left hemisphere, particularly the frontal region, mediates movement of the head to the right.
As noted earlier, the left hemisphere appears dominant
for speech perception in the newborn.
Motility and Power
The quantity, quality, and symmetry of motility and
muscle power are the parameters of interest. Prechtl
and co-workers182-184 combined videotape and electrophysiological methods to describe the postnatal development of motor activity in the term infant. In the
first 8 weeks, movements with a writhing quality
predominate; in the period from 8 to 20 weeks,
‘‘fidgety’’ movements are prominent, and after the
latter period, rapid large-amplitude antigravity and
intentional movements (‘‘swipes’’ and ‘‘swats’’) are
prominent. In general, preterm infants exhibited similar patterns of motor development when they attained
comparable postmenstrual ages, albeit with minor
delays in tone and quality of movements.175,184-186
Prechtl and others184,187-199a emphasized that the quality of spontaneous movements in preterm and term
infants are of major importance with regard to the
status of the central nervous system.
Saint-Anne Dargassies, using less sophisticated
techniques, described the developmental changes in
132
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Means and SD of head positions face to
right
120
100
80
60
left
40
20
0
20
40
60 %
7
9
10
5
17
50
3
4
2
8
1
11
15
6
Figure 3-8 Preference of head position. Mean ± SD percentage of
minutes from all observations in which each infant (series of 14) had
face to right or left side. (From Prechtl HF, Fargel JW, Weinmann HM,
Bakker HH: Postures, motility and respiration of low-risk pre-term
infants, Dev Med Child Neurol 21:3-27, 1979.)
motility in the preterm infant.56 At 28 weeks, movements tend to involve the entire limb or trunk and
have either a slow rotational component or a
fast, large-amplitude characteristic. By 32 weeks of
gestation, movements are seen to be predominately
flexor, especially at the hips and knees, often occurring
in unison.56 Although head turning is present,
neck flexor and extensor power are negligible, as
judged by complete head lag on pull to sit or when
the infant is held in the sitting position. By 36 weeks,
the active flexor movements of the lower extremities are
stronger and often occur in an alternating rather than
symmetrical fashion. Flexor movements of the upper
extremities are prominent. For the first time, definite
neck extensor power can be observed. When
the infant is supported in the sitting position,
the head is lifted off the chest and remains upright
for several seconds. By term, the awake infant is particularly active if stimulated with a gentle shake. Limbs
move in an alternating manner, and neck extensor
power is still better. Neck flexor power becomes
apparent; when the infant is pulled to a sitting position
with firm grasp of the proximal upper limbs, the head is
held in the same plane as the rest of the body for several
seconds.56
The importance of a fixed developmental program in
motor development is suggested by the similarities
in such development when comparing (at the same
postmenstrual age) the fetus, the premature infant,
and the term infant, albeit with minor exceptions.56,63,171,175,182,184,186,191,200,201 The similarities
outweigh the rather small differences.
Tendon Reflexes and Plantar Response
Tendon Reflexes. Tendon reflexes readily elicited in
the term newborn are the pectoralis, biceps, brachioradialis, knee, and ankle jerks. I have considerable
difficulty obtaining triceps jerks in term infants.
Most of these reflexes are elicitable but less active in
preterm infants. The knee jerk is often accompanied
by crossed adductor responses, which should be
considered normal findings in the first months of
life (<10% of normal infants demonstrate crossed adductor responses after 8 months of age).202 Useful
techniques for eliciting tendon reflexes in the newborn
and the frequency and intensity of the reflexes in
preterm and term infants are illustrated in Figures 3-9
and 3-10.203
Ankle clonus of 5 to 10 beats also should
be accepted as a normal finding in the newborn
infant, if no other abnormal neurological signs are
present and the clonus is not distinctly asymmetrical.
Ankle clonus usually disappears rapidly, and the existence of more than a few beats beyond 3 months of age
is abnormal.
Plantar Response. The plantar response is usually
stated to be extensor in the newborn infant.201,204
This result clearly relates to the manner in which
the response is elicited. Using drag of thumbnail
along the lateral aspect of the sole, Hogan
and Milligan205 observed bilateral flexion in 93 of
100 newborn infants examined. My colleagues and
I206 observed a similar result in 116 (94%) of
124 infants. In contrast, Ross and associates,207 using
drag of pin or pinstick, observed a predominance of
extensor responses, with flexion in only about 5% of
patients.
When evaluating the neonatal plantar response, it is
necessary to consider at least four competing reflexes
leading to movements of the toes. Two reflexes that
result in extension are nociceptive withdrawal (often
accompanied by triple flexion at hip, knee, and ankle)
and contact avoidance (elicited best by stroking the
dorsum of the foot, which often occurs inadvertently
when holding the foot to elicit the plantar response).
Two responses that lead to flexion are plantar grasp and
positive supporting reaction (both elicited by pressure on
the plantar aspect of the foot). Because of these competing reflexes and the relative inconsistency of
responses, I consider the plantar response to be of limited value in the evaluation of the newborn infant when
attempting to determine the presence of an upper
motor neuron lesion.