Bạn đang xem bản rút gọn của tài liệu. Xem và tải ngay bản đầy đủ của tài liệu tại đây (43.93 MB, 1,109 trang )
Chapter 4
TABLE 4-1
Cerebrospinal Fluid Findings in HighRisk Newborns
Findings
Term
9±6
0–29
Protein Concentration
(mg/dL)
Mean
Range
90
20–170
115
65–150
Glucose Concentration
(mg/dL)
Mean
Range
52
34–119
50
24–63
74
55–105
Data from Sarff LD, Platt LH, McCracken GH Jr: Cerebrospinal fluid
evaluation in neonates: Comparison of high-risk infants with and
without meningitis, J Pediatr 88:473-477, 1976; based on
87 term infants and 30 low-birth-weight infants (<2500 g), 95 of
whom were examined in the first week of life.
Under all circumstances, assessment of the CSF in the context of
the clinical setting and the clinical features is most important.
Red Blood Cell Counts in High-Risk
Newborns
Determination of ‘‘normal’’ values for RBC in neonatal
CSF is hindered by the relatively high incidence of germinal matrix-intraventricular hemorrhage, usually
clinically silent in the preterm infant (see Chapter 11)
and by the likelihood that the process of birth is associated with minor amounts of subarachnoid bleeding.
In the study of Sarff and co-workers,11 the median
value for RBC count was 180, with a very wide range
(0 to 45,000) (Table 4-3). A similar value was obtained
for premature infants in that study. In both the term
and preterm infants, the most common value (mode)
for RBC count was 0. However, in the report of
Rodriguez and co-workers,12 although a median value
of 112 was observed, the mean was 785, and 20% of
CSF samples had more than 1000 RBC/mm3 (see Table
4-3). These infants were smaller (<1500 g), but
SARFF AND CO-WORKERS*
n = 87
Median: 180
Range: 0–45,000
Preterm infants (<2500 g)
n = 30
Median: 112
Range: 0–39,000
RODRIGUEZ AND CO-WORKERS{
Preterm infants (<1500 g)
n = 43
Mean: 785
>1000 cells/mm3
in 20% of samples
*Data from Sarff LD, Platt LH, McCracken GH Jr: Cerebrospinal fluid
evaluation in neonates: Comparison of high-risk infants with and
without meningitis, J Pediatr 88:473-477, 1976.
{Data from Rodriguez AF, Kaplan SL, Mason EO Jr: Cerebrospinal fluid
values in the very low birth weight infant, J Pediatr 116:971-974,
1990.
ultrasonographic examinations were said to show no
evidence of intracranial hemorrhage. However, exclusion of minor subarachnoid hemorrhage by cranial
ultrasonography is not reliable.
The aforementioned data indicate that the finding of
more than 100 RBCs/mm3 in the newborn is common
and that in very-low-birth-weight infants, values greater
than 1000 occur in a substantial minority in the absence
of apparently clinically significant intracranial hemorrhage. Again, the importance of combinations of findings
is important in the evaluation of the CSF for intracranial
hemorrhage. Thus, the addition of xanthochromia and
elevated protein concentration in CSF strongly raises
the possibility of a more substantial and, clinically
speaking, more important intracranial hemorrhage.
This issue is discussed in more detail in Chapter 10.
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL STUDIES
Several specialized neurophysiological techniques have
been particularly valuable in further defining the neurological maturation of the newborn. Moreover, some of
these studies are commonly used in neurological diagnosis. In this section, I particularly discuss brain stem
auditory evoked responses, visual evoked responses,
somatosensory evoked responses, and EEG (including
Cerebrospinal Fluid Findings in High-Risk Infants of Low Birth Weight (<1500 g)
Postconceptional
Age (Weeks)
26–28
29–31
32–34
35–37
38–40
Red Blood Cell Counts (Cells/mm3) in
Cerebrospinal Fluid of High-Risk Newborns
Term infants
Cerebrospinal Fluid/Blood
Glucose (%)
Mean
81
Range
24–248
TABLE 4-2
TABLE 4-3
Preterm
White Blood Cell Count
(cells/mm3)
Mean ± standard deviation 8 ± 7
Range
0–32
155
Specialized Studies in the Neurological Evaluation
White Blood Cell Count
(cells/mm3 ± SD)
6
5
4
6
9
±
±
±
±
±
10
4
3
7
9
Glucose (mg/dL ± SD)
85
54
55
56
44
±
±
±
±
±
39*
81
21
21
10
Protein (mg/dL ± SD)
177
144
142
109
117
±
±
±
±
±
60*
40
49
53
33
*Values for glucose and protein were significantly greater at 26 to 28 weeks than at subsequent postconceptional ages.
SD, standard deviation.
Data from Rodriguez AF, Kaplan SL, Mason EO Jr: Cerebrospinal fluid values in the very low birth weight infant, J Pediatr 116:971-974, 1990; based
on 43 infants, some studied more than once, approximately 80% studied after the first week of life.
156
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
V
I
III
IV
VI
II
Figure 4-1 Brainstem auditory-evoked response, major wave
forms. The responses obtained with several sequential trials
were superimposed. The complete response, with the seven
definable waves, is not observed in the newborn (see text for
details). (From Starr A, Amlie RN, Martin WH, Sanders S:
Development of auditory function in newborn infants revealed
by auditory brainstem potentials, Pediatrics 60:831-839, 1977.)
+
100 nV
–
2 msec
amplitude-integrated EEG). The most widely used of
these neurophysiological techniques, EEG, also is discussed regarding seizures in Chapter 5.
Brain Stem Auditory Evoked Responses
Electrophysiological investigation of the auditory
system in the newborn has focused on brain stem
evoked responses. However, cortical auditory evoked
responses have been studied, as have visual and somatosensory evoked responses (see later sections), through
computer-averaged EEG recordings obtained over
the scalp after graded stimuli. Such cortical responses
have been described in premature and full-term
infants,17-33 and these responses demonstrate
that peripheral auditory stimuli are transmitted to the
primary and secondary auditory cortex of the temporal
lobe in the newborn period. Magnetoencephalography
has been used to define the maturation of cortical
evoked responses from 27 weeks of gestation to term
in 18 fetuses.34,35 This work is noteworthy for detection
of a decrease in latency from 300 milliseconds at 29
weeks of gestation to 150 milliseconds at term. This
novel and noninvasive technique thus not only extends
insights into the maturation of auditory cortical
areas during the last trimester of human gestation, but
also demonstrates the applicability of magnetoencephalography to study of the fetus. Nevertheless,
measurement of cortical auditory evoked potentials
has been difficult to adapt to routine clinical circumstances, in part because the amplitude and latency
of the observed responses vary with the infant’s level
of arousal and in part because of the expense of the technology (magnetoencephalography). In contrast, major
attention has been paid to the earlier potentials generated from subcortical structures after auditory stimulation (i.e., the brain stem auditory evoked response).
Major Wave Forms and Anatomical Correlates
The brain stem auditory evoked response reflects the
electrical events generated within the auditory pathways from the eighth nerve to the diencephalon and
is recorded by electrodes placed usually over the mastoid and vertex. The stimulation is usually a click or
pure tone administered at a relatively rapid rate.
The signal is amplified, then summed in a computer,
and finally displayed by an X-Y plotter for measurements of the latency and amplitude of the various
components.23,24,36-42 To avoid movement and
other artifacts, the infant is studied preferably during
sleep. The complete response consists of seven
components, designated consecutively by Roman
numerals (Fig. 4-1).24,36,39 Studies in animals and in
adult humans indicate that the waves derive from
sequential activation of the major components of the
auditory pathway.37,38,43-45 Thus, wave I represents
activity of the eighth nerve, wave II the cochlear
nucleus, wave III the superior olivary nucleus, wave
IV the lateral lemniscus, and wave V the inferior colliculus. The precise origins of waves VI and VII remain
to be established, but these waves probably are generated in the thalamus and thalamic radiations, respectively. Brain stem auditory potentials have been well
defined in the newborn infant,23,24,36,39-41,46-61
although all seven components are not observed (see
later discussion).
Developmental Changes
Impressive ontogenetic changes in the brain stem auditory response have been described.23,24,36,39,47,49,
50,54,55,61-75 The most reproducible and easily definable
components are waves I, III, and V; the last is sometimes fused with wave IV. Waves II, VI, and VII have
generally been too variable to allow systematic study.36
The latencies of the most prominent components (I, III,
IV to V) decrease as a function of gestational age, with a
maximal shift occurring in the weeks before 34 weeks
of gestation (Fig. 4-2). Moreover, an increase in amplitude and a decrease in threshold of the response occur
with increasing gestational age.
The decrease in latency of wave I with maturation
indicates improvement in peripheral processing of the
auditory stimulus. Whether this effect is at the level of
middle ear conduction, the cochlear receptors, or the
Chapter 4
Brain stem evoked responses
60 dB 10 cs (N = 74)
Maturation of Wave I & V
157
Specialized Studies in the Neurological Evaluation
8
(IV-V)-I
9.5
Latency (msec)
7
9.0
8.5
Wave V
Latency (msec)
8.0
6
5
1.10 msec
7.5
4
24
0.85 msec
7.0
2.5
Wave I
0.95 msec
0.30 msec
2.0
30-31
32-33
34-35 36-37 38-39
Gestational age (wk)
28
40
30
34
32
36 38
Conceptional age (wk)
42
44
Figure 4-3 Decrease in latency of brain stem portion (i.e., time difference between waves I and IV to V) of brain stem auditory evoked
response as a function of gestational age. (From Starr A, Amlie RN,
Martin WH, Sanders S: Development of auditory function in newborn
infants revealed by auditory brainstem potentials, Pediatrics 60:831839, 1977.)
3.5
3.0
26
40-41
42-43
Figure 4-2 Decrease in latencies of major waves of neonatal brain
stem auditory evoked response as a function of gestational age.
(From Despland PA, Galambos R: The auditory brainstem response
[ABR] is a useful diagnostic tool in the intensive care nursery,
Pediatr Res 14:154-158, 1980.)
transduction of cochlear receptor potentials into eighth
nerve activity (i.e., wave I) is unknown.
The time difference between wave I and the wave IVV complex depends on transmission through the brain
stem auditory pathway (i.e., the brain stem transmission
time), and this maturational change is shown in Figure
4-3. Whether this decrease in latency relates to changes
in nerve conduction velocity or synaptic efficiency is
unknown. Active myelination within this brain stem
system is occurring during the time period shown in
Figure 4-3 (see Chapter 2); however, distinct postnatal
changes in brain stem auditory evoked responses69,70,76
appear to occur after rapid myelination has ceased.
Detection of Disorders of the Auditory
Pathways
Abundant findings indicate the value of brain stem
auditory evoked response studies in detecting disorders of the auditory pathways in the newborn
infant.23,24,36,39-41,53,56-61,77-104 Definition of such disorders depends on detection of responses that are
abnormal in threshold sensitivity, conduction time
(i.e., latency), amplitude, or conformation. In neonatal
studies, deficits in threshold sensitivity and latency
have been the most valuable. The general principle is
that a lesion at the periphery (middle ear, cochlea, or
eighth nerve) results in a heightened threshold and a
prolongation of latency of all the potentials, including
wave I, whereas a lesion in the brain stem causes longer
latencies of only those waves originating from structures distal to the lesion, with wave I spared. The essential features of these two basic abnormal patterns of
brain stem auditory evoked responses observed in neonatal patients are depicted in Table 4-4.
Abnormalities of the evoked response in neonatal
neurological disease are to be expected, in part because
of the known neuropathological involvement of the
following: the cochlear nuclei, the inferior colliculus,
other brain stem nuclei, and the cochlea itself by
hypoxic-ischemic insult (see Chapter 8); the cochlear
nuclei, inferior colliculus and, perhaps, the cochlea or
eighth nerve by hyperbilirubinemia (see Chapter 13);
the eighth nerve by bacterial meningitis (see Chapter
21); the cochlea and eighth nerve by congenital viral
infections (see Chapter 20); and the cochlea by intracranial hemorrhage (see Chapter 11) (Table 4-5).
Indeed, brain stem evoked response audiometry has
been used to describe peripheral and central disturbances in infants with congenital cytomegalovirus
infection, hyperbilirubinemia, bacterial meningitis,
asphyxia, persistent fetal circulation, aminoglycoside
TABLE 4-4
Two Basic Abnormal Patterns of
Brain Stem Auditory Evoked
Responses in Neonatal Disease
SITE OF DISORDER
Response
Characteristic
Periphery
Brain Stem
Threshold (wave I)
Wave I latency
Wave V latency
I–V interval
Elevated
Prolonged
Prolonged
Normal
Normal
Normal
Prolonged
Prolonged
158
TABLE 4-5
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Probable or Proven Examples of Neonatal
Neurological Disease with Abnormal Brain
Stem Auditory Evoked Responses
Neurological Disorders
Relevant Neuropathology
Hypoxic-ischemic
encephalopathy
Hyperbilirubinemia
Cochlear nuclei, inferior colliculus, cochlea
Cochlear nuclei, inferior colliculus, cochlea, eighth
nerve
Eighth nerve
Cochlea, eighth nerve
Cochlea
Bacterial meningitis
Congenital viral infection
Intracranial hemorrhage
or furosemide administration, trauma to the cochlea or
middle ear, and still undefined complications of low
birth weight.23,24,36,39-41,56-61,75,77,79-93,96,97,100,102,104,
105,105a The particular importance of combinations of
these factors in the genesis of permanent deficits has
been emphasized. Moreover, neonatal defects may be
transient. For example, in one large study (N = 92) of
term asphyxiated infants, 35% exhibited brain stem
auditory evoked response deficit (increased threshold)
in the first 3 days of life, but only 10% had abnormalities
at 30 days.59 Among preterm infants with birth weight
less than 1500 g who were studied at term, 14% had
evidence of a peripheral impairment (increased threshold), 17% a central impairment (prolonged brain
stem latencies), and 4% a combined impairment, for a
total of 27%.57
Hearing Screening
Use of the brain stem auditory evoked response as a
screening device for hearing impairment in the neonate
has become extremely common and is the norm in
many countries.24,39,94,98,99,102,106,107 The importance
of early identification of infants with hearing impairment is based on the realization that acquisition of
normal language and of social and learning skills
depends on hearing.24,39,89,98-102,106,108-114
The most commonly recommended screening procedure, for preterm infants, consists of testing the infant
just before hospital discharge, or at least as close to
40 weeks after conception as possible, when he or she
is medically stable, and preferably in a room separate
from the neonatal unit. Term infants are often tested at
any point before discharge.24,98,99,106,110,112 The initial
screening procedure has consisted of conventional
brain stem auditory evoked response, automated auditory evoked response, or transient evoked otoacoustic
emission technique. The latter detects signals generated by cochlear outer hair cells in response to acoustic
stimulation. This technique is faster and less expensive
than evoked response audiometry. However, the
method does not detect retrocochlear abnormalities
(e.g., auditory nerve disease). Infants who fail this test
are retested by auditory evoked response study, often
an automated study.106,107,112 Notably, retesting in the
neonatal unit has been shown to result in reduction of
failure rate by at least 80%.114 The incidence of failure
of either screening test at the time of hospital discharge
is relatively high, the actual value depending on the
population studied. For low-birth-weight infants
tested at term, failure rates as high as 20% to
25% are common.24,94 Retesting infants after test failure usually is carried out after several weeks or later,
often after discharge. With this approach, many infants
are lost to follow-up. Because most neonates who fail
the first screening procedure exhibit normal responses
at the time of the retest,24,39,49,52,53,94,98,99,106,110,114 the
initial failures are likely transient, reversible disturbances, or false-positive results. For example, in one
large series of more than 16,000 infants, retesting in
the neonatal unit after early test failures resulted
in an 80% reduction in failure rate by discharge.114 In
certain high-risk groups, the importance of later testing
is emphasized by the report of hearing deficits developing in the first months of life, after normal results in the
neonatal period.81,82,112
Visual Evoked Responses
Cortical Response
The visual evoked response refers to the electrical response,
recorded usually by surface electrodes on the occipital
scalp, to a standardized stimulus, the most common of
which is a light flash of graded intensity and frequency.
Flash visual evoked responses are recorded in response
to red light-emitting diodes in goggles placed over the
infant’s eyes or in an array placed about 6 inches in front
of the infant’s eyes.115-117 The fully developed response
is complex, but the first two prominent waves consist of
first a positive and then a negative deflection. The positive deflection is attributed to postsynaptic activation at
the site of the predominant termination of visual afferents, and the negative deflection is attributed to secondary synaptic contacts in the superficial cortical layers.118
Two features of the response are studied: the quality of
the wave form and the latency between stimulus and
recorded response. With flash visual evoked responses,
variability in latencies can lead to difficulties in
interpretation.
An alternative and generally preferable stimulus
for visual evoked responses, particularly for study
of visual acuity, is a shift (reversal) of a checkerboard
pattern (i.e., pattern-shift or pattern-reversal visual
evoked response).37,38,115,119-121 This stimulus results
in responses with less variable latencies than those
obtained with a light flash stimulus. Although the technique has been used in the newborn,37,38,115,119,120,122
including the preterm newborn,119,120,123 experience
remains limited, in part because obtaining optimal
data requires that the newborn ‘‘fix’’ on the visual display. However, reliable data have been obtained, and
this technique should prove adaptable to the newborn
for wider use.
Developmental Changes
The ontogenetic changes of the visual evoked response
in the human newborn have been well established.115117,119-121,124-136 A prolonged negative slow wave can be
identified as early as 24 weeks of gestation, and this wave
ultimately is replaced by the more discrete negative wave
Chapter 4
Specialized Studies in the Neurological Evaluation
159
VEP latency vs gestational age
350
24 wk
320
N300
27 wk
+
5 μV
Latency (msec)
22 wk
290
260
230
29 wk
+
12 μV
200
24
27
30
33
Gestational age (wk)
36
VEP amplitude vs gestational age
300
P200
37 wk
40 wk
+
2.5 μV
0
200
400
600
msec
800
1000
Figure 4-4 Visual evoked potentials to light-emitting diode stimulation from newborn infants, showing no recordable response at
22 weeks of postconceptional age (PCA), the emergence of N300
at 24 weeks of PCA, the late positivity following the N300 (usually
c 450 milliseconds), which is evident from about 27 weeks onward,
and then little change until closer to term. The P200 then emerges
and becomes the most prominent wave in the normal term newborn’s
visual evoked potential. (From Taylor MJ: Visual evoked potentials. In
Eyre JA, editor: The Neurophysiological Examination of the Newborn
Infant, New York: 1992, MacKeith Press.)
noted earlier (Fig. 4-4). The positive wave appears
between approximately 32 and 35 weeks of gestation,
and by 39 weeks the visual evoked response is quite
well defined. As with the components of the brain
stem auditory evoked response, the latencies of both
the positive and negative waves of the visual evoked
response decrease in a linear fashion with increasing
maturation (Fig. 4-5). This evolution in the quality
and latency of the response corresponds well with the
behavioral studies of visual function noted in Chapter 3.
That this ontogenetic change is principally an inborn
program is suggested by the finding that differences
between infants born at term and healthy premature
infants grown to term are small,137 and these differences
dissipate completely shortly after the time of term.138
Although the anatomical substrate for the ontogenetic
changes is undoubtedly complex, the major maturational changes correspond to the period of rapid dendritic development in the visual cortex and myelination
of the optic radiations (see Chapter 2).
240
Amplitude (μV)
+
5 μV
180
120
60
0
24
27
30
33
36
Gestational age (wk)
Figure 4-5 Visual evoked potential (VEP) latency and amplitude
versus gestational age (ga) in weeks in 86 preterm infants. The
regression line and the 95% confidence interval are indicated.
(Latency = 370.7 À 3.4 Â ga; amplitude = 440.4 À 11.2 Â ga.)
(From Pryds O, Trojaborg W, Carlsen J, Jensen J: Determinants of
visual evoked potentials in preterm infants, Early Hum Dev 19:117125, 1989.)
Detection of Disorders of the Visual Pathway
Attempts to use the neonatal visual evoked response as a
measure of cerebral disturbance have been promising.22,23,41,115,117,121,132,135,136,139-147 Premature infants
with serious hypoxemia secondary to respiratory distress
syndrome were shown to lose visual evoked responses
during the insult and to regain the responses with restoration of normal blood gas levels (Fig. 4-6).133,139
Similarly, impairment of the visual evoked response
has been demonstrated in the first day after asphyxia in
term infants, and the severity of the abnormality correlated well with poor neurological outcome.41,135,142,147
In a study of 36 term infants who experienced ‘‘birth
asphyxia’’ and who were studied by serial assessment
of visual evoked responses, 14 of 16 infants with
normal responses in the first week of life were normal
on follow-up, and all 20 with abnormal responses
persisting beyond the first week died or were ‘‘significantly handicapped’’ at 18 months of age.147 A related
observation in fetal and neonatal lambs indicates
160
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
is demonstrated as a function of increasing gestational
age, with the most marked decrease in the last several
weeks before term (Fig. 4-7). The decrease in latencies
in the somatosensory evoked response relates principally to myelination evolving in the brain stem and
thalamocortical components of the somatosensory
system (see Chapter 2). As with visual responses, maturation of somatosensory evoked responses does not
differ significantly between term infants and healthy
premature infants studied at term.
The potential clinical value of the somatosensory
evoked response in the study of disorders of the sensory
85
9.45
10.15
11.00
12.10
–100 μV
12.40
msec
200
400
600
N 13/AL (msec/min)
11.30
70
60
50
40
800
Somatosensory Evoked Responses
Somatosensory evoked responses have been studied
most extensively in the newborn by electrical stimulation of the median nerve at the wrist or, less commonly,
the posterior tibial nerve above the ankle or in the popliteal fossa and recording of the computer-averaged
responses on the EEG over the contralateral parietal
scalp.41,125,151-169a Constant primary components
appear at approximately 27 weeks of age (i.e., a few
weeks earlier than the comparable visual response),
and the response pattern of the mature newborn
appears at 37 to 38 weeks of age. As with the visual
evoked response, a linear decrease in response latency
N 19 lat (msec)
14
12
10
30
20
10
50
N 19 peak lat (msec)
the sensitivity of the visual evoked response to asphyxial
insult.148 Abnormalities of the visual evoked response
have also been described in infants with posthemorrhagic
hydrocephalus (see Chapter 11),115,149,150 a finding probably reflecting the disproportionate dilation of the
occipital horns of the lateral ventricles and consequent
affection of the geniculocalcarine radiations. Moreover,
improvement in latencies was documented immediately after ventricular tap,150 as well as over a prolonged
period after placement of ventriculoperitoneal shunt.149
The data suggest that the determination of visual evoked
responses in the neonatal period provides important
information concerning cerebral function, effects of
interventions, and outcome.
N 13 peak lat (msec)
FLASH
Figure 4-6 Visual evoked potentials after single flashes from a premature infant recorded at 9.45 hours after birth, at 10.15 hours after
intubation and start of mechanical ventilation, at 11.00 hours during
a 5-minute episode of hypoxia (partial pressure of arterial oxygen
[PaO2] = 2.9 kPa), and at 11.30, 12.10, and 12.40 hours during
recovery. (From Pryds O, Greisen G, Trojaborg W: Visual evoked potentials in preterm infants during the first hours of life, Electroencephalogr
Clin Electrophysiol 71:257-265, 1988.)
40
30
20
10
34
38
42
46
50
Postconceptional age (wk)
Figure 4-7 Percentile curves (P97, P50, P3) for the postconceptional age of 36 to 48 weeks estimated from the somatosensory
evoked potentials of 103 normal infants. (From Bongers-Schokking
JJ, Colon EJ, Hoogland RA, Van den Brande JL, et al: Somatosensory
evoked potentials in term and preterm infants in relation to postconceptional age and birth weight, Neuropediatrics 21:32-36, 1990.)
Chapter 4
pathway in the newborn relates to the finding that by
appropriate placement of surface electrodes closest to
the presumed generator sources, the several components of this pathway can be monitored, as demonstrated in older patients.37,38,168,169a Thus, conduction
through peripheral nerve, plexus, dorsal root, posterior
column, gracile or cuneate nucleus, (contralateral)
medial lemniscus, thalamus, and parietal cortex is
involved. Considerable experience in adult patients
indicates that evaluation of the somatosensory evoked
response can provide insight into disease at various
levels along this pathway. Clear application to the newborn has been demonstrated for spinal cord trauma and
myelodysplasia,170-172 as well as for a variety of cerebral
abnormalities, including hypoxic-ischemic insult in the
term or preterm infant, intraventricular hemorrhage and
posthemorrhagic ventricular dilation in the preterm
infant, hypoglycemia, and congenital hypothyroidism.41,162-164,167-169,173-179 Because the thalamus, the
thalamocortical projections, and the parietal cortex of
the somatosensory system (areas interrogated by
measurement of the somatosensory evoked potentials)
are all involved in and are contiguous to other
areas related to cognitive and motor development,
persistent abnormalities in these potentials in the neonatal period may be predicted to be associated with
subsequent deficits. Indeed, in a study of 63 term
and preterm infants, two thirds of whom were
‘‘asphyxiated,’’ abnormal somatosensory evoked
responses in the neonatal period in 14 were associated
with abnormal neurological outcome at 1 year in
11 infants; normal responses in the neonatal period
occurred in 26, with normal neurological outcome in
23.180 Prognostic value in the preterm infant is clearly
less than in the term infant.41,166
Electroencephalogram
Normal Development
Maturation of spontaneous EEG recorded activity has
been studied in considerable detail in newborn infants,
often in combination with studies of sleep
states.138,169,181-214 The theme apparent from the
studies of specific sensory evoked responses recurs:
with increasing gestational age, impressive elaborations
of measurable function occur, characterized principally
by more refined organization, and whether infants
are born at term or grow to term after uncomplicated
premature delivery has little or no effect on
these developments. The normal development of
EEG patterns in the neonatal period is evaluated best
in relation to sleep states. In general, active sleep is the
predominant sleep state in the newborn and consists
of greater than 70% of definable sleep time in the smallest premature infants and approximately 50% in
term infants. In the following discussion, I review
the major changes in EEG over approximately the
12 to 13 weeks before term. Development of EEG is
considered best in terms of the continuity of
background activity, the synchrony of this activity,
and the appearance and disappearance of specific
Specialized Studies in the Neurological Evaluation
161
waveforms and patterns (i.e., EEG developmental
landmarks) (Table 4-6).200
27 to 28 Weeks. Activity at this developmental stage is
characteristically discontinuous, with long periods of
quiescence (see Table 4-6).200 The activity that does
interrupt the quiescence occurs in generalized, rather
synchronous bursts (Fig. 4-8). No distinctions between
wakefulness and sleep or change in EEG to external
stimulus such as loud sound (i.e., reactivity) are
apparent.
29 to 30 Weeks. The discontinuity of the EEG
continues at this stage, but now the activity is asynchronous (see Table 4-6; Fig. 4-9).200 The principal developmental landmark is the appearance of delta brushes
(i.e., delta waves of 0.3 to 1.5 Hz with superimposed
fast activity in the beta range, usually 18 to 22 Hz),
sometimes also called beta-delta complexes (Fig. 4-10).200
These complexes appear in the central regions at this
stage. In addition, temporal bursts of theta activity
(4 to 6 Hz) are a second developmental landmark of
this period (see Fig. 4-10). These bursts occur independently in left and right temporal areas; their sharp configuration has provoked the term saw-tooth pattern.
31 to 33 Weeks. At this stage, continuous activity
appears during active (or rapid eye movement) sleep
(see Table 4-6).200 Moreover, although EEG is generally
asynchronous, a degree of synchrony appears in active
sleep. The presence of more synchrony in active sleep
than in quiet sleep persists throughout the developmental period of the third trimester. The delta brushes now
become more prominent in occipital and temporal areas
and are apparent particularly in quiet sleep. The temporal theta bursts of earlier stages give way to temporal
alpha bursts, still, however, exhibiting the sharp sawtooth pattern (Fig. 4-11).
34 to 35 Weeks. The degree of continuity in the EEG
now increases further and is apparent in the awake state
as well as in active sleep (see Table 4-6).200 In concert,
the degree of synchrony increases in the awake and
active sleep states. Of the developmental EEG landmarks, the delta brushes now exhibit considerably
higher-voltage, faster activity. The temporal theta
bursts disappear during this phase. Frontal sharp wave
transients (i.e., sharp waves appearing as an abrupt
change from background) become apparent (Fig. 4-12)
and are characteristic for their diphasic, synchronous,
and generally symmetrical configuration. These normal
waves should be distinguished from higher-voltage,
unilateral, persistently focal, periodic, or semirhythmic
sharp waves, which are abnormal and indicative of focal
disease (see later discussion). At this stage, EEG
becomes ‘‘reactive’’ to external stimuli. Most commonly, this reactivity consists of a generalized attenuation of the amount and voltage of delta activity,
especially apparent in response to sound.
36 to 37 Weeks. The degree of continuity and of synchrony in the awake and active sleep states is still more
162
Unit II
TABLE 4-6
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
Developmental Aspects of Electroencephalographic Activity
SYNCHRONY OF
BACKGROUND
ACTIVITY{
CONTINUITY OF
BACKGROUND
ACTIVITY*
Postconceptional
Age (Weeks)
Awake
Quiet
Sleep
Active
Sleep
Awake
Quiet
Sleep
Active
Sleep
27–28
29–30
À
D
D
D
D
D
À
0
++++
0
++++
0
31–33
D
D
C
+
+
++
34–35
C
D
C
+++
+
+++
36–37
C
D
C
++++
++
++++
38–40
C
C
C
++++
+++
++++
EEG Developmental
Landmarks: Specific
Waveforms and Patterns
1. ‘‘Delta brushes’’ in central
regions
2. Temporal theta bursts (4–6 Hz)
3. Occipital slow activity
1. ‘‘Delta brushes’’ in occipitaltemporal regions
2. Temporal alpha bursts replace
theta bursts (33 weeks)
3. Rhythmic 1.5-Hz activity in
frontal leads in transitional
sleep
1. Extremely high-voltage
beta activity during ‘‘delta
brushes’’
2. Temporal alpha bursts
disappear
3. Frontal sharp-wave
transients
1. Central ‘‘delta brushes’’
disappear
2. Continuous bioccipital
delta activity with
superimposed 12–15-Hz
activity during active
sleep
1. Occipital ‘‘delta brushes’’
decrease and disappear
by 39 weeks
´
2. Trace alternant pattern
during quiet sleep
*C, continuous activity; D, discontinuous activity.
{
0, total asynchrony; ++++, total synchrony.
EEG, electroencephalographic.
Adapted from Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of
Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
C3T3
C4T4
1 sec 100 μv
T3F1
T4F2
Figure 4-8 Electroencephalogram of a male infant of 27 to 28 weeks of postconceptional age. The bursts of generalized, bilaterally synchronous activity separated by prolonged periods of electrical quiescence are characteristic of this age. Selected sample from a 16-channel recording.
(From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical
Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
C3T3
C4T4
T3F1
1 sec 100 μv
T4F2
´
Figure 4-9 Trace discontinu pattern in a male infant with a postconceptional age of 29 to 30 weeks. Selected sample from a 16-channel
recording. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of
Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
C3T3
C4T4
T3F1
T4F2
T3C3
C3Cz
CzC4
C4T 4
1 sec 100 μv
EOG
Resp
(Nasal)
Resp
(Chest)
ECG
Figure 4-10 Electroencephalogram of a female infant with a postconceptional age of 30 to 32 weeks. Left, Brief bursts of 4- to 6-Hz waves of
sharp configuration occurring asynchronously in the temporal regions. Right, Beta-delta complexes in the central and temporal regions. Selected
sample from a 16-channel recording. ECG, electrocardiogram; EOG, electro-oculogram. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P:
Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York:
1990, Raven Press.)
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
C3T3
C4T4
T3F1
1 sec 100 μv
T4F2
Figure 4-11 Brief bursts of 8- to 9-Hz waves occurring bilaterally in the temporal regions in a female infant with a postconceptional age of 32
to 33 weeks. Selected sample from a 16-channel recording. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In
Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
164
Unit II
NEUROLOGICAL EVALUATION
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
1 sec 100 μv
C4O2
Figure 4-12 Diphasic, bilaterally synchronous and virtually
symmetrical, frontal sharp waves in transitional sleep in a male
infant with a postconceptional age of 36 weeks. Selected sample
from a 16-channel recording. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM,
Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley
TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed,
New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
apparent (see Table 4-6).200 At this stage, for the first
time, EEG in the awake state differs from that in sleep
by the presence of low-voltage activity, with a mixture
of activities in the alpha, beta, theta, and delta frequency bands (Fig. 4-13). Of the developmental EEG
landmarks, the delta brushes in the central region disappear. These are replaced by similar complexes in the
occipital regions (i.e., bioccipital delta with superimposed 12 to 15 Hz activity, which appears during
active sleep).
38 to 40 Weeks. At this stage, continuous activity now
appears in quiet sleep as well as in active sleep and the
awake state (see Table 4-6).200 A considerable degree of
synchrony is present in all states. The occipital delta
brushes disappear, and the interesting trace alternant pat´
tern becomes apparent in quiet sleep (Fig. 4-14).
This quasiperiodic tracing is characterized by periods
of 3 to 15 seconds of generalized voltage attenuation,
interrupted by higher-voltage, generally synchronous
´
activity. Trace alternant should not be confused with
the more ominous burst-suppression pattern (see later
discussion).
Clinical Application
The following sections focus on the application of conventional EEG in the clinical arena. The procedure
requires skilled technicians and experienced interpreters of the tracing. Definitive assessment of EEG
abnormalities in the premature and term newborn
requires conventional multichannel EEG. In the following discussion, I review the principal EEG abnormalities of both the premature and term newborn
(Table 4-7), except for the EEG correlates of neonatal
seizures (see Chapter 5).
Disordered Development. Delineation of abnormalities of EEG maturation clearly requires awareness of
the normal developmental changes described in the
previous section. Impairment of development level of
more than 3 weeks, according to reported gestational
age, is clearly abnormal.200,215 Such disturbances are
often but not necessarily associated with other EEG
abnormalities, and the degree of disturbance may
differ according to the state of the infant.215-217
Abnormalities may be apparent only in quiet sleep,
and thus this sleep state should be included in the
EEG evaluation of the newborn.200 Disturbed development of the EEG does not provide specific information
regarding disease process and may reflect either an
acute or a chronic disturbance.
Depression and Lack of Differentiation. Depression
of background activity, especially of the faster frequencies, often accompanied by lack of differentiation
(i.e., disappearance of the normal multiple frequencies),
is common after generalized insults, especially hypoxicischemic insults (Fig. 4-15).200,213,215,218,219 Other EEG
abnormalities are also often present. In addition to
hypoxia and ischemia, other bilateral cerebral insults
may produce this EEG pattern, particularly acutely
(e.g., bacterial meningitis, encephalitis, and metabolic
disorders). Persistence of this EEG pattern is an unfavorable prognostic sign.
Excessively Discontinuous Activity. The development of continuous or intermittent discontinuity of
EEG in the term infant is a very common feature
of all neonatal encephalopathies. The most extreme
of these discontinuous tracings is the burst-suppression
pattern, which is associated with a very high likelihood
of an unfavorable outcome. However, burst-suppression tracings account for the minority of excessively
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
T3C3
C3Cz
1 sec 100 μv
CzC4
C4T4
Figure 4-13 Typical awake pattern in a male term infant, characterized by a mixture of activities in the alpha, beta, theta, and delta frequency
bands. Selected sample from a 16-channel recording. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn. In
Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
Chapter 4
Specialized Studies in the Neurological Evaluation
165
F1C3
F2C4
C3O1
C4O2
C3T3
C4T4
1 sec
T3F1
100 μv
T4F2
´
Figure 4-14 Trace alternant pattern in a male term infant. (From Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P: Electroencephalography of the newborn.
In Daly DD, Pedley TA, editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.)
discontinuous neonatal EEGs. Recent data indicate
that relatively simple analysis of the latter tracings is
highly useful in predicting outcome (see later).
The burst-suppression pattern can be considered the
most severe of the excessively discontinuous tracings
just described. The EEG pattern is characterized by
long periods (usually > 10 seconds) of marked
depression of background activity (voltage < 5 mV),
alternating with shorter periods of paroxysmal bursts,
usually lasting 1 to 10 seconds and characterized by
high-voltage (75 to 250 mV) theta and delta activity
with intermixed spikes and waves (Fig. 4-16). This
EEG pattern should be distinguished from the
normal discontinuous tracing of the very immature
´
premature infant and from the trace alternant of quiet
sleep of the infant beyond 36 weeks of gestation. Two
important distinguishing features of the burst-suppression pattern are persistence of the discontinuous pattern throughout the tracing and nonreactivity (i.e., no
change in the EEG with arousal attempts and painful or
other stimuli). A burst-suppression pattern that is
‘‘reactive’’ (i.e., is altered by external stimuli) is not so
uniformly associated with a poor prognosis as the
nonreactive variety described here.220-225 The poor
prognosis of burst-suppression EEG is illustrated
by the data depicted in Table 4-8.226 Bacterial meningitis is the one disturbance in which I have seen a
TABLE 4-7
Major Electroencephalographic
Abnormalities of the Premature and
Term Newborn
Disordered development
Depression: lack of differentiation
Excessively discontinuous activity, including burstsuppression pattern
Electrocerebral silence
Unilateral depression of background activity
Periodic discharges
Multifocal sharp waves
Central positive sharp waves
Rhythmic generalized or focal alpha activity
Hypsarrhythmia
Data primarily from Hrachovy RA, Mizrahi EM, Kellaway P:
Electroencephalography of the newborn. In Daly DD, Pedley TA,
editors: Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, 2nd
ed, New York: 1990, Raven Press.
favorable outcome, despite the finding of a burstsuppression EEG during the acute disease. In
the group described in Table 4-8, the one infant with
a favorable outcome also had acute bacterial
meningitis.
Analysis of the duration of the predominant interburst
interval (IBI) has proven to be a relatively simple
means of quantitation of excessively discontinuous tracings
in the term infant, and the analysis has major prognostic implications.227 Thus, of 43 term infants (70% with
hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy) with an excessively
discontinuous EEG, 10 parameters regarding the burst
and IBIs were quantitated and compared with outcome.
One parameter, the IBI duration that accounted for
more than 50% of all IBI durations (classified into
10-second blocks), also known as the predominant IBI
duration, predicted an unfavorable neurological outcome with high specificity (Table 4-9). Thus, IBI durations lasting longer than 30 seconds were invariably
associated with an unfavorable outcome, and those
with a duration of more than 20 seconds were
associated with an unfavorable outcome in 92%. Of
the 43 discontinuous tracings, only 7 (16%) exhibited a
burst-suppression pattern, as defined earlier. Thus, the
predominant IBI duration, readily quantitated at the bedside, was highly effective and, critically, applicable to the
large group of excessively discontinuous tracings in
term newborns with encephalopathy.
Electrocerebral Silence. Electrocerebral silence, of
course, is the worst end of the continuum from depressed EEG through excessive discontinuity and
burst-suppression pattern. Persistence of electrocerebral silence for 72 hours or more is indicative of cerebral death.228,229 However, electrocerebral silence
indicates cerebral cortical death and not necessarily
brain stem death; if clinical evidence of persistent
brain stem failure is not present, survival is possible,
although in a persistent vegetative state (see Chapter 9).
Unilateral Depression of Background Activity. A
marked voltage asymmetry between hemispheres of
background rhythms that persists in all states is clearly
different from the normal shifting asymmetries, particularly during quiet sleep (Fig. 4-17). Such persistent
unilateral depressions of background activity are